Waking Effectiveness of
Audible, Visual and Vibratory Emergency Alarms
on People of all Hearing Abilities
Jacqueline Du
Bois, Erin Ashley, Michael Klassen, and Richard Roby
Combustion Science & Engineering
Alerting devices of all kinds have been marketed to deaf and
hard of hearing people as capable of providing levels of
fire protection equal to what the audible alarm provides for
people who are fully hearing able. This study, sponsored by
the National Institute of Deafness and other Communication
Disorders, quantified the effectiveness of commercially
available devices by how well the alerting stimulus can
awaken individuals of varying hearing abilities. Waking
people from sleep is of significant importance because the
majority of fire deaths (nearly 50%) occur between the hours
of 11:00pm and 6:00am when most people are sleeping1.
People recruited to participate in this sleep study were
divided among three hearing classifications: profoundly
deaf, hard of hearing, and fully hearing able. The alerting
devices examined included a standard audible smoke detector
(3100 Hz, >75 dB), a low frequency audible smoke detector
(450 Hz, > 75dB), a strobe light (110 candela, 1Hz), a bed
shaker, and an intermittent bed shaker (both 0.14-0.19 RSS).
Sleep stage of the study participants was carefully
monitored such that the impact of brain activity during
sleep could be clearly distinguished in the study results.
Sleep stage, hearing ability, device salience, age, and time
to awaken were investigated in order to determine their
influence on an individual’s likelihood to arouse
involuntarily.
Of the five distinct expressions of sleep, this study
focused on three stages in particular: Stage 2, Delta, and
REM. In Stage 2, the body temperature decreases, the heart
rate slows, and the brain prepares for deep sleep. During
Delta sleep, also know as Stages 3-4, brain activity slows
making this the deepest stage of sleep. Stage 5 or REM
sleep, also known as the dream stage, is characterized by an
increase in brain activity to levels similar to the lightest
sleep stage; however, the major voluntary muscle groups of
the body remain paralyzed.2 Over the course of a night’s sleep, a person descends
sequentially through Stages 1 through 4, then ascends back
through the stages with REM replacing Stage 1 after the
initial onset of sleep. The brain’s traversal of the stages
continues with the depth of each cycle lessening.3 The duration of each stage is not equally
distributed across each sleep period. In fact, Stage 4
occurs predominantly during the first third of the night,
while REM is more prevalent during the last third3.
An initial hypothesis of this study has been that the
prevalence of fire deaths during the late evening hours may
be related to the predominance of the deep sleep in sleep
patterns of adults who retire around midnight.
The experiment was designed such that the sample size of
each hearing classification would be consistent with a
binomial distribution (either success or failure in
awakening) with success rates near or greater than 80%. Of
the 111 subjects selected for the study, 32 were profoundly
deaf, 45 were hard of hearing, and 34 were fully hearing
able. Hearing classifications were based on analysis
of participants’ audiograms. People categorized as deaf had
no hearing 90 dB or less over the sound frequency range of
500 Hz to 8000 Hz. Hard of hearing participants’ average
hearing ability fell between 20 dB – 90 dB over the range of
250 Hz – 8000 Hz. Fully hearing subjects had hearing of 20
dB or less across the frequency spectrum of 250 –8000 Hz.
The tests were conducted under controlled conditions at a
nationally recognized sleep laboratory, Sleep Services of
America. Individual alerting devices were activated at
random for two minutes once subjects reached stable states
within Stage 2, Delta, or REM sleep. Only three awakenings
per subject were permitted. If arousal did not occur,
additional devices were sequentially activated --with
two-minute recovery intervals between activations-- until
the subject awakened or experienced a vacillation in sleep
depth. In this regard, multiple alarm activations could be
achieved without violating the three awakenings per subject
limit. Coherence upon awakening was determined by subjects’
capacity to answer a series of simple questions. Study
participants were not required to evacuate the facility upon
alerting.
The percentage of standard audible smoke detector alarm
activations yielding awakenings in hearing able subjects
regardless of sleep stage was 92%. Hard of hearing and deaf
subjects experienced awakening potentials with the same
device of only 57% and 0% respectively. The low frequency
alarm yielded better results for the hard of hearing,
illuminating the impact of frequency on audibility; however,
deaf subjects were still disadvantaged. Awakening
potentials for the low frequency audible alarm were 11% for
deaf subjects, 92% for hard of hearing, and 100% for hearing
able.
The strobe’s effectiveness was slightly elevated for the
deaf, 57%, but poor overall with ratings of 34% for hard of
hearing, and 32% for hearing able. These results clearly
indicate that for people with hearing loss, strobes, though
recommended by the National Fire Protection Association and
the Americans with Disabilities Act, are not functionally
equivalent to audible alarms.
The tactile bed shakers offered a much greater effectiveness
for all of the hearing echelons in addition to demonstrating
an awakening effectiveness for the deaf subjects equal to
that of hearing able subjects exposed to audible alarms.
Awakening effectiveness for the continuous bed shaker was
93% for deaf, 82% for hard of hearing, and 92% for hearing
able subjects. The intermittent bed shaker provisioned 100%
effectiveness for all study participants irregardless of
hearing ability, underlining the fact that an intermittent
tactile signal is sufficiently salient and recognizable as
an emergency alarm, even more so than the continuous tactile
alert. Weighting the device results according to the US
population hearing demographics permitted a generalizing of
the study results to the entire population.4 A ranking of the devices based on this statistic is as
follows: 100% for the intermittent bed shaker, 91% for the
continuous bed shaker, 90% for the low frequency audible
alarm, 83% for the standard audible alarm, and 33% for the
strobe.
With one exception, the measured awakening effectiveness of
the devices dropped when subjects were in Delta sleep. Only
the intermittent bed shaker yielded equal, indeed 100%
awakening effectiveness, across all sleep stages for people
of all hearing abilities. Taking into account subjects’
hearing levels and all of the devices, awakening potentials
were 59% in Delta, 65% in REM, and 69% in Stage 2. The
variance in these data, however, is not sufficient to
establish statistical significance to this phenomenon of
fewer awakenings from Delta. The inability to grant
statistical relevance to this despite experimental data has
been demonstrated by other researchers5, 6. Nonetheless, it is apparent
that there is a trend towards a decreased likelihood
of awakening during the Delta sleep stage. And, the
intermittent bed shaker’s ability to evoke awakening equally
across all sleep stages should not be ignored.
Other parameters found pertinent to awakening potentials
were the age of study participants and the time to awaken.
The intermittent bed shaker elicited responses among all
test subjects for all presentations of the pulsed tactile
signal. Of the other devices tested, study participants
over the age of 60 alerted to 7% - 25% fewer alarm
presentations than their 18 – 60 year old study
counterparts. Expanding this statistic to the United States
population in general, people over the age of 60 effectively
awaken to 26-40% fewer alarms than younger people. Analysis
of the latency period preceding awakening showed that 88% of
the time, the subjects who awakened reached consciousness
30- seconds after the alarms were activated. 96% of the
time, those who awakened reached consciousness after 60
seconds. Beyond 60-seconds, the chance of awakening dropped
to 8%. In a similar set of experiments with audible alarms
and fully hearing subjects, Bruck showed that 75% of
awakenings occurred within 30 seconds of alarm activation,
87% occurred within 60 seconds, and the likelihood of
awakening dropped to 13% after 60 seconds. In the field of
fire safety, time to awaken is related to the amount of time
one has to escape a building. If flames are spreading
towards an alarm mounted along an escape route, a person may
have only a limited amount of time after awakening to escape
along that route before thermal conditions silence the
alarm, and subsequently make exiting along that path
impossible. Typically, audible smoke detectors degrade when
the air immediately surrounding them reaches 92 C; and they
are no longer able to produce sound at 114C 7 . Silencing of an alarm
positioned adjacent to the fire’s room of originated can
occur within a minute during a typical house fire.
In conclusion, this study has served to provide a
quantitative measure to the awakening effectiveness of the
alerting devices commercially available for the deaf and
hard of hearing populations. Strobes, promulgated by the
National Fire Protection Association as functional
equivalents to the audible smoke alarm, are actually 53%
less effective than the standard smoke detector according to
the weighted average rankings. The only devices equivalent
to the audible detector for both deaf and hard of hearing
people were the bed shakers, the intermittent bed shaker in
particular. While sleep stage was not a definitive
contributor to awakening potential, age was. This data has
also shown that individuals of advanced age are at a greater
risk of succumbing to fires at night because of their
overall difficulty awakening. This is of particular
importance because individuals over 65 compose the fastest
growing portion of the population 8.
Combustion Science & Engineering, Inc. has leveraged this
data to propose important recommendations to the National
Fire Protection Association that will appear in the 2006
National Fire Code (NFPA 72) and Guide on Alternative Approaches to Life Safety (NFPA 101A) 9.
___________________________
1Ahrens, M., “The U.S. Fire
Problem Overview Report: Leading Causes and Other Patterns
and Trends,” NFPA Publication, 2003.
2 “Sleep states”, http://www.sleepdisorderchannel.net/stages, September 2004.
3 Pezoldt, V.J. and van Cott, H.O.,
“Arousal from Sleep by Emergency Alarms: Implications from
the Scientific Literature,” National Bureau of Standards
Consumer Sciences Division publication, NBSIR
78-1484(HEW),1978.
4 Lucas, J.W., Schiller, J.S. and
Benson, V. “Summary Health Statistics for U.S. Adults”,
Vital Health Statistics 10 (218):5, 34-37 (2004). According
to the report, 83% of the United States population over 18
years of age is hearing able, 14% is hard of hearing, and 3%
is profoundly deaf.
5 Bruck, D. and Horasan, M.,
“Non-arousal and Non-action of Normal Sleepers in Response
to a Smoke Detector Alarm,” Fire Safety Journal, 25:125-139
(1995).
6 Bonnett, M.H. and Johnson,
L.C., “Relationship of arousal threshold to sleep stage
distribution and subjective estimates of depth and quality
of sleep,” Sleep, 1:161-168 (1978).
7 Experimental tests performed at
Combustion Science & Engineering, Inc., Columbia, MD
2000-2002.
8 U.S. Census Bureau 2004, “U.S.
Interim Projections by Age Sex, Race and Hispanic Origin,” http://www.census.gov/ipc/www/usinterimproj/, September
2004.
9 Recommendations based on
information generated from this study will appear in NFPA
72: Appendix 11.3.6, and NFPA 101A: 6.4.6.3
Back to home page |